17 Comments

My view is that if you have an intervention and the underlying problem actually gets worse than the intervention is probably not that useful.

Psychiatric interventions are just not the useful. Maybe we should be prescribing Hinduism and Bollywood movies.

Expand full comment

Thanks for nice posting i will visit again.

Expand full comment

As a former psychiatrist with no skin in the game, I have to say that Yvain has done a great job of shedding some light on the staggering misinformation that Whitaker has been putting out.

You cannot understand historical psychiatric data without a knowledge of how the field has evolved, and even if you have that understanding, conclusions are difficult to draw. Whitaker ignores all that, and makes assertions that are misleading and at times embarrassing. He frequently claims to compare 'data' on illnesses like schizophrenia and bipolar disorder from the 1950s to the present. What he doesn't admit to understanding is that what was diagnosed as 'schizophrenia' in the 1950s is substantially different that what is diagnosed as schizophrenia today.

In the 1950s, 'schizophrenia' often included almost every possible mental illness that involved psychosis, emotional unresponsiveness, or a thought disorder. Many psychiatrists considered it a variety of diseases and therefore a garbage-can term. Some psychiatrists in the 1950s still preferred the term 'dementia praecox.' Since there was no effective treatment for what we now know as schizophrenia until Thorazine was introduced in 1954, the specific diagnosis simply didn't matter (and of course Thorazine treats more than schizophrenia, so even an effective treatment didn't clear things up completely). Basically much of what we now classify as bipolar disorder, severe depression, drug/toxin reactions, some forms of epilepsy, and so on were all diagnosed as 'schizophrenia.'

The diagnosis of schizophrenia have evolved dramatically over the last 60 years, and it continues to evolve. Whitaker seems to assume that the diagnosis has been utterly consistent, and any variation in the rates or typical progression of 'schizophrenia' must be caused by psychiatry. He has it exactly backwards. The data he claims to reveal simply doesn't exist.

Bipolar disorder has it's own overlapping and complex history, making comparisons with past rates virtually impossible. Until lithium was accidentally discovered to effectively treat manic depression, many people with manic depression were misdiagnosed as schizophrenic/dementia praecox. Lithium was first reported in the literature in 1949, and of course it was years after that that it's effective use became widespread. The term 'bipolar disorder' was first introduced in 1952, and wasn't widely used for a time after that. In general 'manic depression' means more-or-less the same as 'bipolar disorder,' but until relatively recently, there was little standardization in what any diagnostic terminology meant.

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual was created to deal with this problem, and first published in the 1950s. If you compare the original DSM with the subsequent revisions, you will see constant fine-tuning of diagnostic criteria, as well as the creation of new diagnoses (e.g., Bipolar Disorder type 2, Bipolar Disorder NOS). What this means is that looking at the 'data' from a study from the 1950s, and another from the 1970s, and a third from 2008, and assuming that the terms and diagnoses are equivalent is an exercise in futility (unless one has a book to sell). Whitaker no doubt points to published studies to back up his conclusions, but he's on a fool's errand.

It's for these reasons that this grotesquely idiotic statement can apparently be made with a straight face: "Though bipolar disorder was relatively rare just a half-century ago, reported rates of it have increased more than a hundredfold, to one in 40 adults." Of course it was rare! The diagnosis simply didn't exist.

Regarding the data on psychiatric diagnoses, there is also the matter of intentional misdiagnosis, which will skew much of that data. Some psychiatrists look hard to find an alternative diagnosis, for example, when dealing with a high-functioning patient with schizophrenia or major depression, since the diagnosis is so stigmatizing. On the other hand, there are financial incentives to come up with a more 'serious' diagnosis like 'Bipolar Disorder,' since this will get treatment authorized much more readily than something like an 'Adjustment Disorder' or a milder form of depression. Sadly, some treatment professionals will exaggerate a diagnosis (even as they perform appropriate treatment of the actual diagnosis) for reimbursement reasons. Finally, there is the fact that the majority of psychiatric diagnoses are made my non-psychiatrists (either Family Practice doctors or non-MD therapists), and their rate of accuracy isn't always very good. A lot of this diagnostic data is collected, especially by insurance companies, but the data is almost worthless in drawing conclusions about incidence rates.

The bottom line is that picking and choosing studies to compare from across 60 years of psychiatric literature, and attempting to compare psychiatric diagnoses, and treatment outcomes is an extraordinarily dicey proposition. We still have a pervasive misunderstanding of mental illness in our society, and Whitaker is actively contributing to that, to the detriment of patients and patient's families. It's a shame that both the ignorant and the intellectual elite share in the fear of the vague 'evils' of psychiatry, and are so quick to write off the entire field.

And how pathetic is it that Yvain, primarily using Wikipedia (!), can poke so many holes in Whitaker's claims. Robin, I'm glad that Yvain has made you look twice at Whitaker's claims, but wasn't a little fact checking in order before you wrote this? This kind of misinformation does real harm.

Expand full comment

OK, you've persuaded me to doubt; I've added to the post.

Expand full comment

"But I’m not clear on if you are disagreeing with him on the key claim that what was once mostly a temporary condition has turned into a chronic condition."

I didn't want to comment on that because I don't know too much about the history of schizophrenia. But looking it up on Wikipedia:

"From the outset, dementia praecox [the old name for schizophrenia] was viewed by Kraepelin [its discoverer] as a progressively deteriorating disease from which no one recovered. However, by 1913, and more explicitly by 1920, Kraepelin admitted that although there seemed to be a residual cognitive defect in most cases, the prognosis was not as uniformly dire as he had stated in the 1890s. Still, he regarded it as a specific disease concept that implied incurable, inexplicable madness......modifying his previous more gloomy prognosis in line with Bleuler's observations, Kraepelin reported that about 26% of his patients experienced partial remission of symptoms."

So long before the discovery of antipsychotics, only 26% of patients have even partial remission. Although I don't know what criteria Kraepelin used for his study or whether there have been more recent studies using the same criteria, modern psychiatry does view schizophrenia as a disease that often remits as its sufferers grow older, and one study quotes a 33% recovery rate with more people getting better but not recovering completely.

I see no evidence that there has been any deterioration in prognosis since Kraepelin's time, and there are staggeringly more schizophrenics who have been successfully integrated into the community.

Expand full comment

Very good post from Yvain. I suppose at this point it would take a while for Robin to prepare an adequate response to the evidence Yvain has marshalled. But I'd be interested to see that response!

Expand full comment

Well if the author got his facts dead wrong, he would of course draw incorrect conclusions. But I'm not clear on if you are disagreeing with him on the key claim that what was once mostly a temporary condition has turned into a chronic condition.

Expand full comment

Cross-national comparisons: your article talks of an "epidemic" of eg schizophrenia in the developed world which it blames on psych medications, but all my sources (see for example Eaton and Chen 2007) disagree. Incidence of schizophrenia per 1000 people per year is .1 in Honolulu (the only US city that made it onto the study), .15 in Manchester (representative British city), .05 in Vancouver (representative Canadian city), compared to .35 in rural Chandigarh, India, .6 in Madras, and .45 in Brazil. Here the incidence of schizophrenia appears to be up to three times higher in the developing world than in the developed.

Your article's point on greater recovery rates in the developing world is well-taken and does have real data in support of it, but it's still controversial and the latest studies suggest it may be an artifact. In particular, if the worst schizophrenics die in the developing world, but survive in the developed world, then we will find that on average, living schizophrenics in the developing world have milder disease. Studies that control for this fail to find the same effect, or get mixed results. See Cohen, Patel, Thara, and Gureje: "Questioning an Axiom: Better Prognosis for Schizophrenia in the Developing World?"

There are also other major differences in schizophrenia between developed and developing nations: catatonic schizophrenia is ten times more common in developing nations, hebephrenic is three times more common, and acute onset is twice as common (all as percent of total cases). This means that there are serious epidemiological differences between developing- and developed- world schizophrenia from the onset of the disease, as would be expected in a psychiatric disease across two wildly different cultures.

As I said in my previous post, there are many, many diseases which differ between developed and developing countries in currently mysterious ways. The multiple sclerosis incidence rate is two hundred times higher in Canada than in Zimbabwe. Type 1 diabetes (the type supposedly not related to diet) is forty times higher in Finland than in Tanzania. Diseases that can be directly related to diet, like heart disease or stroke, are even more dramatic.

In the course of all this, to take one disease that *may* differ between developed and developing countries, and blame it on medication without any evidence, is wildly jumping the gun.

A more reasonable suggestion would be to look at different populations within a country, which brings us to your quoted portion, about Harrow and Thorazine. You say:

"Before the introduction of Thorazine in the 1950s, Whitaker asserts, almost two-thirds of the patients hospitalized for an initial episode of schizophrenia were released within a year, and most of this group did not require subsequent hospitalization. Over the past half-century, … schizophrenia has come to be seen as a largely chronic, degenerative disease. "

But the numbers tell a different story. From the Wikipedia article on chlorpromazine (the chemical name for thorazine):

"The development and use of antipsychotic drugs like chlorpromazine was one of the forces that propelled deinstitutionalization, the systematic removal of people with severe mental illness from institutions like psychiatric hospitals and their reintegration into the community[citation needed]. In 1955 there were 558,922 resident patients in American state and county psychiatric hospitals. By 1970, the number dropped to 337,619; by 1980 to 150,000, and by 1990 between 110,000 and 120,000 patients."

Then there's the Harrow study cited about the differential recovery rates of schizophrenics. This study is not randomized (!!!!!) That means that they just found schizophrenics who already were (or weren't) on antipsychotics, and included them in the study. Why would schizophrenics not be on medication? The obvious reason would obviously be because their schizophrenia isn't very bad and they don't need it. In this case, the obvious reason is the correct reason. According to the text of Harrow's study, which I have in front of me:

"The results suggest that the subgroup of schizophrenia patients not on medications was different in terms of being a self-selected group having better earlier prognostic and developmental potential."

The only reason not to shrug and forget all about this study is that there are very few good long-term studies on schizophrenia, so someone should probably replicate it with better selection criteria. It does not seem impossible to me that there might be a long-term detrimental effect of "typical antipsychotics" like the ones in Harrow's study, although it should be noted that modern "atypical antipsychotics" are completely different drugs with a completely different mode of action and it would be surprising if by a strange coincidence they ALSO made the disease worse. However, uncertainty of long-term effects aside, there are lots of good shorter-term studies, which show (quoting Dixon, Lehman, and Levine):

"The Baldessarini et al. (1990) and Davis et al. (1993) reviews find overwhelming evidence that conventional antipsychotic agents reduce the risk of relapse of the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Based on 44 placebo-controlled studies of antipsychotics with a total of 3,939 subjects and an average followup period of 9.8 months, Baldessarini et al. (1990) found that the rate of symptom exacerbation on placebo was 55 percent versus 14 percent on active medication (mean daily dose = 397 CPZ equivalents), a 3.9-fold overall sparing of relapse attributable to medication."

All of this stuff like looking for selection bias and so on are things any epidemiology student would learn in a first-year course, which is one reason why I hate it when people with no epidemiological training read a couple of studies and think they have discovered a massive conspiracy in the medical world. As an economist, imagine the grating feeling you get when a sociologist or philosopher uses the broken window fallacy or says raising tariffs would increase economic growth and so determines that all economists must be part of a massive conspiracy because they disagree. Then please try to have a little more respect for doctors, epidemiologists, biostatisticians, and so on.

Expand full comment

From Yale Law & Policy Review: "In November 2006, the American Medical Association (AMA) adopted a new ethics provision categorically prohibiting doctors from using placebos deceptively. In so doing, the AMA shifted the legal landscape, making it almost certain that courts will decide that placebo deception violates informed consent requirements. "

My father, a US primary care doctor, says that his practice has always assumed that if they tried it they would be sued and it would be an open-and-shut guilty verdict. There's some gray area around suggesting treatments that probably won't work for scientific reasons but would make good placebos (eg antibiotics for viral infections) but just prescribing sugar pills is a total legal disaster.

Expand full comment

How about if we pulled the anti-depressants' FDA certs, and moved them to "woulda-banned stores? :-P

Yvain: excellent post!

Expand full comment

The part about Africans doing better is old information and may have been true even before anti- psychotics. Africans were are said to act crazier than Western schizophrenics but were not locked up and were thought of a being kind of magic. The point about reduced hospitalization seems askance,as most of the insane asylums were shut down because of the efficacy of anti-psychotic drugs. I wouldn't jump on one guy's band wagon just because he is counter cultural. Interesting, but will it go anywhere.Most of these kind of things don't.

Expand full comment

I agree that those are both interesting suggestive evidence (perhaps Thorazine is already accepted as harmful and no longer prescribed, as Yvain claims; still, it's weak evidence to suspect today's psych-med practices). Yvain's reply to those two bits of evidence is just the generic "maybe some other difference is causing it" - agreed; it's only weak evidence.

I had the same doubt as Yvain about the 1998 WHO study that may have just been making the point that (long-term) antidepressant use is correlated with depression - it's too close in wording to a ridiculously vacuous claim (as Yvain lampoons it) for the writer to not clarify how it was non-trivial.

For some reason I trusted too much the rest of the quoted material (perhaps because I wrongly assume Robin is endorsing the claims he quotes) in spite of this clear warning. I appreciate Yvain's deeper examination that revealed other problems.

Expand full comment

"the legal system doesn’t currently support ability to prescribe placebo."

What do you mean by that? Half of US physicians surveyed report having prescribed them. Wikipedia says that half of Danes prescribe them monthly. Maybe you mean that they are limited to untreatable illness?

In any event, there are many legal systems and an opportunity to try this in the most flexible one.

Expand full comment

Actually, Robin's posts on the effectiveness of medicine are some of his best work -- in fact some of the best researched and most important blog posts anywhere. The fact that he gores many sacred cows with them is not a surprise. He has put a lot of thought and research into this topic.

Expand full comment

The most interesting parts to me were the cross-national comparisons, which you dismiss, and:

Before the introduction of Thorazine in the 1950s, Whitaker asserts, almost two-thirds of the patients hospitalized for an initial episode of schizophrenia were released within a year, and most of this group did not require subsequent hospitalization. Over the past half-century, ... schizophrenia has come to be seen as a largely chronic, degenerative disease.

Expand full comment