Silent Line-Videos Pick Music Winners

[In] classical music competitions, … nearly all participants — including highly trained musicians — were better able to identify the winners of competitions by watching silent video clips than by listening to audio recordings. … The effect held up even in high-level international competitions, which often feature not only top performers, but also highly trained musicians as judges. (more; HT Hugh Parsonage)

I give lots of quotes from the original study below the fold. Ordinary people and classical piano experts were rewarded for picking the winner from the top three candidates in ten prestigious international classical music competitions. People said and bet that they would guess better using sound only, but they in fact guessed better using video only, even when the video was reduced to line drawings like:

PianoLines

They guessed worse when they had both audio and video. When they rated videos on various keywords, the word that best predicted winners was “passion.”

This strongly suggests that people are reluctant to admit to themselves how much the passion and energy of motion of pianists influences their evaluation of such pianists. I recently puzzled over why people pay so much more attention to lead singers relative to backup singers when by most accounts the musical skill difference, if any, is very small. (Here’s a recent movie on this.) This new result suggests those usual musical skills are only a minor part of what people want from a singer — lead singers get most of the attention because they give most of what folks want – a vivid passionate attractive character to relate to.

I suspect we’d find similar results hold for novelists and academics – people think they rate them mostly on content, but even experts usually put more weight on style, i.e., on energy and control relative to plot, setting, characters, problem choice, analysis, etc.

Overall this fits into the homo hypocritus framework, as it seems less licit or admirable to like musicians, novelists, or academics mainly because we like to affiliate with people with lots of energy and control. We prefer instead to think that what we like is nice music or words, and that artists are just instruments to get us those things.

Those promised quotes:

These findings suggest that there may be gaps between what we say we use to evaluate performance and what we actually use. People may be unlikely to recognize or admit that visual displays can affect their judgment about music performance, particularly in a domain in which other signals are deemed to be more indicative of quality. …

In highly competitive arenas such as music, competitions emerge as one launching pad for establishing careers. … . Given different versions of competition performances, 1,164 participants in total were asked to identify the actual competition winners. These choices were then compared against the established outcomes, previously decided by panels of expert judges. …

Suppose that you have the chance to win cash bonuses if you can guess who won a live music competition. You may choose the type of recording you think would give you the best chance at winning the prize. You can select sound recordings, video recordings, or recordings with both video and sound. Which recordings do you choose? In experiment 1, participants were asked to make exactly that decision and bet their study earnings on their choices. As expected, 58.5% chose the sound recordings, significantly more so than the 14.2% who chose video recordings. … Despite a “tax” levied on selecting the recordings with both video and sound, 27.4% still chose those recordings. …

In experiments 2–5, the top three finalists in each of 10 prestigious international classical music competitions were presented to participants. Given such difficult decisions, untrained participants should fare no better than chance (33%) in identifying the winners of these competitions. In fact, even expert interrater agreement tends to be moderate, hovering at an average of 67%. … In experiment 2, novice participants were presented with both video-only and sound-only versions of 6-s clips of the top performances. … with silent video only recordings, participants were significantly above chance (52.5%). … With sound-only recordings, they were significantly below chance (25.5%) at identifying the winners. …

Experiment 3 tested judgment when more information was available, and presented participants with video-only, sound-only, or video-plus-sound versions of the performance clips included in experiment 2. Participants performed below chance with sound-only recordings (28.8%), and at chance with video-plus-sound recordings (35.4%). However, with silent video-only recordings, 46.4% of novices were able to identify the winners. …

In experiment 4, 96.3% of domain-expert participants reported that the sound mattered more for their evaluations. Despite musicians’ training to use and value sound in their evaluations, only 20.5% of experts identified the winners when they heard sound-only versions of the recordings. However, 46.6% did so upon viewing silent video clips. … In experiment 5, 82.3% of professional musicians cited sound as the most important information for judgment. However, when provided sound, only 25.7% of experts were able to identify the actual winners. … With video-only stimuli, musicians performed significantly better than chance (47.0%) … When provided with stimuli with both video and sound, experts were again at chance at 29.5%.

Experts were not significantly different from novices in their judgments of music performance. …

[In] Experiment 6 … recordings were distilled to their most basic representation as outlines of motion (Fig. S2). After seeing these 6-s silent clips of the three finalists, participants were asked to identify the actual winners. Participants were significantly better than chance (48.8%) at identifying the outcomes. … demographic cues such as race and sex … [and] physical attractiveness … did not significantly impact professional judgment in these competitions. …

In experiment 7, 262 participants were … asked to identify the most confident, creative, involved, motivated, passionate, and unique performer in each set of three finalists in the competitions. … Creativity, involvement, motivation, passion, and uniqueness were significantly more salient through visual cues rather than through sound. … Through silent videos, those selecting “the most passionate contestant” identified the actual winners at rates significantly higher than chance (59.6%). … Involvement (53.1%), motivation (52.8%), creativity (44.6%), and uniqueness (43.6%) also contributed.

Experts in particular reported a severe lack of confidence in their judgment when they were assigned to the video-only recordings, not knowing that their approximations of the actual outcomes would be superior under such constrained conditions. (more)

GD Star Rating
loading...
Tagged as:
Trackback URL:
  • komponisto

    This phenomenon is well known in the context of public speaking/classroom lecturing (dynamic speakers who say nothing of substance are rated highly); I seem to recall at least one rather famous experiment along these lines.

  • http://juridicalcoherence.blogspot.com/ Stephen Diamond

    These were “prestigious” musical competitions, which suggests a much-reduced range (and variance) of competence in music production. On the other hand, the range of competence in energy and passion is apt to be extremely large.

    When the raters misjudged what is important to them, it was probably because the effect of restriction of range is counter-intuitive.

    Another reason your account is unconvincing is you rely on the assumption that energy and passion are high status. Where does this assumption come from? Consider as counter-example high-status British aristocrats, who are supposed to look bored and listless.

    • IMASBA

      “These were “prestigious” musical competitions, which suggests a much-reduced range (and variance) of competence in music production.”

      An important point: when the range in competence is so small it’s just not just the judges who are fools, the people who believe even a perfectly rational judge would reliably pick a “champion” are fools. Competitions with only a small number of events and that only allow for a single winner are usually not something you want to rely on.

      The fault doesn’t just lie with the judges, maybe not with the judges at all, the judges only know music, but they’ve spent their entire lives being told by people who should know better (because of their math credentials) that successful people usually become successful via having “super-powers,” i.e., very unusual abilities, at least within some context. Are the judges hardwired to overestimate their own judging abilities, are they too trusting in supposed experts who tell them superhuman champions exist, are they hardwired to believe in superhuman champions? The answer is not as simple as Hanson makes it look here.

  • IMASBA

    Is this news? This is what the concept of the popular talent show “The Voice” is based on (it’s a talent show like any other, except the judges don’t get to see the contestants).

  • Guest_bro

    Novelists? I don’t think the fact that people consume and rate literature (non-fiction) on the basis of its style is inconsistent with the ‘accepted’ intentions or principles of recreational reading, which is what reading a novel presumably is.

    People, I’m sure, certainly confuse their personal preferences re: style, with an assessment of quality (this is how ‘Breakfast of Champions’ by Kurt Vonnegut is considered a quality novel, which, as even an ardent Vonnegut fan like myself will tell you, it is not).

    But I agree with the hypothesis being posited here. Simplified, it is mankind’s tendency to replace a statement such as “I find Frank Sinatra’s combination of stage presence, physical appearance, and ability to sing reasonably well highly enjoyable, personally” with a statement such as “Frank Sinatra is the BEST singer of the 20th century”, and so on.

    • lemmuslemmus

      Yes. I think our host goes from the meaning of style as not really a part of the core product (e.g., a musician’s body movements) to style in literature, where, of course, style is a core ingredient – I, and many people who would think of themselves as connoisseurs, would say the most important. Robin appears to suggest that problem choice and analysis are more important, but if that’s what you’re looking for, you probably shouldn’t read a work of fiction. (Having said that, I’m aware that there are many ways to write a novel.)

      • Philip Goetz

        Style matters in fiction, but only to editors and writers. I’ve been studying fan-fiction this past year, and I can say confidently that most readers do not give a damn about style, or even grammar. Content rules with readers. There are plenty of fan-fictions written with excellent style, and they are not especially popular. But you don’t need to study fan-fiction to realize that; just check the bestseller lists.

  • Pingback: People Hearing Without Listening | Arcsecond

  • http://priorprobability.com/ F.E. Guerra-Pujol (Enrique)

    I wonder if these findings would also apply to chess …

  • Axa

    Interesting, but you forgot to mention the elephant in the room: TEACHERS.

  • Nancy Lebovitz

    I’d like to see another version of the study comparing ability to predict winners from still photos of the performance, still photos of the musicians while not performing, and videos. I wouldn’t be surprised if the best-looking people have an edge.

    From a different angle: Blind auditions increase the odds of women being hired by classical orchestras.

  • dmytryl

    I seem to be missing some misconception here. Do you think it is logically impossible that people would predict the outcome of a blind musical competition better based on the video only than the audio only, or what?

    > “They guessed worse when they had both audio and video. ”

    Before you unleash the hindsight based pseudo-scientific method on this data, think a little about this passage. It does imply that individuals are in fact judging primarily based on the audio when video is available.

    People have idiosyncratic preferences for specific styles of playing, that’s the thing, the jury averages this out, but one person is stuck picking the player that matches their preference the best. Unless they don’t even hear the player.